See Inside
Genes and personality influence our attitudes toward religion
By Vassilis Saroglou
|
May 5, 2012 |
Image: Bara K. Kristinsdottir/Aurora Photos
In Brief
-
Many people change their religious affiliation during the course of a
lifetime. Overall attitudes toward belief, however, are generally stable
in adulthood.
-
Specific clusters of personality traits correlate highly with particular kinds of religious belief.
-
Although environmental influences play a large role in determining a
person’s religious beliefs during adolescence, genetic factors emerge as
more important in adulthood.
A deep question pervades the debates surrounding religion—whether God
exists, sure, but that one is mighty difficult to answer. Instead we can
ask a related, more approachable query: Why does God exist for some of
us but not for others? Theologians and ministers preach that faith is
preeminently a matter of personal choice. Is it, really?
Not everyone is a believer, of course, nor do we all maintain
allegiance to a single belief system throughout the course of our life.
Almost half of American adults, for example, have changed religious
affiliation at least once during their lifetime, and most do so before
age 24, according to the Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life. Although
religious affiliation may be fluid, once people enter adulthood they
tend to stick with one category, retaining either faith in God or the
absence thereof.
For the most part, people are either religious or atheists because they
were raised that way. Parents, classmates and other trusted figures
impress their views on children and introduce them to a set of rituals
and practices. Later in life those influences hold less power. Several
forces can diminish a person’s religiosity—frequently cited reasons
include the absence of social pressures to be religious or a desire to
distance oneself from one’s family. Personal crises can also spur a
change, prompting some people to convert and others to abandon religion.
Recent research suggests, however, that this is not the whole story. By
studying the correlations among thousands of individuals’ religious
beliefs and measures of their thoughts and behaviors, scientists have
discovered that certain personality types are predisposed to land on
different spots of the religiosity spectrum. Genetic factors account for
more than half of the variability among people on the core dimensions
of their character, which implies that a person’s feelings regarding
religion also contain a genetic component. By analyzing twins, some of
whom share the same DNA, psychologists have begun to collect evidence
for the genetic roots of religiosity. These studies are starting to
explain what makes some of us believers, whereas others end up rejecting
supernatural notions.
Bringing Up Believers
The search for a biological basis for religion has gained wide appeal
as the tools to probe our internal makeup have improved. Numerous
brain-scanning experiments have sought to pinpoint one or another brain
region as being important to the religious experience, prompting
occasional claims that humans are equipped with a “God module,” a part
of the brain that causes us to have religious beliefs. In 2004 a much
hyped book called
The God Gene proposed that a particular gene,
VMAT2, was linked with religiosity. The data supporting that claim,
however, were never published in a peer-reviewed journal, and other
scientists never replicated the purported results.
Discerning how genes lead to behavior is one of biology’s toughest
tasks. Genes make proteins, and figuring out how those proteins give
rise to behaviors, let alone beliefs, pushes at the edges of our
scientific knowledge. What is clear is that genes are not a blueprint;
instead they interact with environmental influences in many complex
ways, twisting fate at every turn. One way to examine the question is to
look at personality characteristics: genes predispose a person to
particular traits, which can manifest as certain behaviors.
The study of personality began almost a century ago, when pioneering
psychologists working in the 1920s and 1930s became inspired by
biology’s orderly classification systems and set out to codify
personality. They started by scanning the dictionary for all the terms
that captured some aspect of a person’s character, producing a list
several thousand items long. Factions of psychologists debated over
which descriptors, and how many of them, were needed to capture the
essential dimensions of personality.
More recently, psychologists have rallied around “the big five,” as
psychologist Lewis Goldberg of the University of Oregon called them in
1981. These five traits—extroversion, neuroticism, agreeableness,
conscientiousness and openness—have been shown to be independent of one
another and to remain stable throughout most of life. In work published
in 1987 Robert R. McCrae and Paul T. Costa of the National Institutes of
Health verified the five factors by administering questionnaires and
collecting self-reports and peer ratings from thousands of people.
Subsequent surveys in many languages and countries have contributed to
the dominance of the five-factor model in personality psychology today.
According to this model, the ways in which individuals’ personalities
differ from one another can be organized along five main dimensions.
People differ in extroversion: extroverts are dynamic, gregarious and
socially warm, whereas introverts are timid and reserved. Neuroticism
refers to a person’s tendency to be anxious, depressed and generally
emotionally vulnerable, as opposed to emotionally stable and positive. A
third facet is agreeableness, which captures whether a person is
empathetic, helpful and trusting of others, as opposed to mean,
individualistic and arrogant. Conscientious individuals are methodical,
self-controlled, and willing to establish goals and work toward
achieving them, whereas those low in conscientiousness tend to be
impulsive and disorganized. Finally, we can differ in openness: whether
we like novel, challenging and complex ideas, experiences and feelings.
Less open individuals prefer to stay within their comfort zone.
Linking Personality and Religion
To find links between a person’s religious beliefs and any other facet
of life, scientists must sift through enormous quantities of data. In
2010 I published an in-depth analysis of 70 previous studies seeking to
link religion and personality with a total of more than 21,000
participants. These papers covered several decades, ages and religions,
although Christianity was most heavily represented. Several of these
reports corroborated self-assessments, with ratings provided by family
members, friends and colleagues.
What those studies revealed is that religious people consistently
differ from low-religious or nonreligious individuals on two
personality dimensions: agreeableness and conscientiousness. The effects
were modest in size: 60 percent of religious versus 40 percent of
nonreligious people are agreeable or conscientious. Yet this correlation
showed up in study after study. It was present in both men and women,
from teenagers to adulthood, and among several cohorts ranging from the
1970s to the present, as well as in a study from the 1940s. We saw this
trend in people of all major religions, not only in Protestant and
Catholic but also in Jewish and Muslim faiths. Several behavioral
experiments bolster the idea that religious individuals tend to display
agreeable and conscientious behaviors. For example, religious people are
inclined to show cooperation in laboratory experiments and to volunteer
in real life. They also endorse healthy lifestyles that reflect
self-control such as low alcohol, drug and tobacco use. Again, these
effects are modest, but the fact that they are pretty consistent across
studies makes them notable.
One could argue that rather than certain types of people being more
likely to become religious, religion might instead instill agreeableness
and conscientiousness in believers. To answer this question,
researchers examined data from the Terman Longitudinal Study, a project
that followed people with high IQs throughout their life. In the early
1920s, when these participants were between the ages of 12 and 18, their
parents and teachers evaluated various aspects of their personalities.
In 2003 Michael McCullough of the University of Miami and his
collaborators found that of the 492 subjects they analyzed, the children
and adolescents rated as more agreeable and conscientious turned out to
be more religious 19 years later than the individuals who were rated
lower on these measures as children. Another analysis published two
years later examined changes in religiosity of Terman study participants
over the course of 50 years. The people who were high in agreeableness
in their early adulthood were more likely to remain believers or even to
become more religious later than those who were less agreeable as young
adults.
These results are in line with personality theory. Personality traits
are already present in early childhood. Later in life they heavily shape
social attitudes, values and identities. It thus becomes clear that
rather than religion making people agreeable and conscientious, it is
personality that determines religiousness.
If further research, especially in Eastern cultures and religions,
confirms this pattern, we may have psychological evidence in favor of
some of the universal functions of religion. Scholars have long
suggested that because religion fosters social cohesion, it may have
played an important evolutionary role by enabling larger groups of
people to band together. These findings on personality traits support
that idea. Agreeableness and conscientiousness together denote a
preference for social harmony and personal order—in other words,
stability.
A Profile of Belief
We can add even more nuance to our personality profiles of believers.
In my 2010 meta-analysis I also reviewed studies that had focused on
either spirituality, which encompasses more modern forms of faith not
necessarily connected to religious institutions, or fundamentalist types
of religion. About 62 percent of those who are high in openness to
experience—and are agreeable and conscientious—are interested in and
involved with spirituality. About the same percentage of people who are
agreeable and conscientious but low on openness tend to become involved
with fundamentalist religious groups.
Openness appears to tune believers
to the kind of faith they end up holding.
An important question is how these clustered traits might relate to
choices in real life or at least to real life as it is modeled in the
lab. In a study I conducted in 2005 at the Université Catholique of
Louvain in Belgium with Isabelle Pichon, we asked Belgian participants
how they would react in several situations in which they could choose to
either offer help or not. Here is one scenario: you are trying to catch
a train when you see a person whose suitcase flies open and from which
the contents scatter. Do you stop to help? We assigned our subjects
randomly to one of two conditions. In one, the person needing help was a
friend, family member or colleague. In the other, the person requiring
assistance was unknown. Our findings were intriguing: the more religious
the participants, the more they expressed willingness to help the
familiar individual but not the stranger. Spiritual subjects, however,
did not distinguish between known and unknown people. They were equally
willing to help in both cases.
We can make further distinctions among types of religiosity. In
collaboration with my graduate student Joanna Blogowska, we replicated
the suitcase scenario with Polish participants in a study published in
2011. We added a second study, in which we examined the willingness to
help either a student in need or a feminist student in the same
situation. It turned out that participants who were high on religious
fundamentalism were not very willing to help unknown people or a
feminist, an individual whom they perceived as threatening to their
values. They did, however, frequently offer to help either a close
acquaintance or a student in need. The participants who were high in
fundamentalism assisted individuals in those latter two categories 66
percent of the time versus exactly half of the time for feminists and
strangers. In other words, those viewed as outsiders were least likely
to receive a helping hand from more conservative believers.
Genes and Environment
These clustered personality traits—and their corresponding
behavior—suggest an underlying genetic component. To investigate this
idea, researchers have contacted hundreds of pairs of twins to assess
their religious beliefs at different points in time. These twin studies
aimed to identify how each of the following variables helped to
determine religiosity—the unique experiences of each twin, the shared
environmental factors of family and environment, and finally,
heritability.
What these studies conclude is that shared environment—namely a
family’s approach to religion—plays a great role, especially during
childhood and adolescence. After that, the picture shifts, the early
environment becomes less potent, and a genetic influence emerges between
the ages of 18 and 25 years.
Let us look a little more deeply at one of these twin surveys. In a
2005 study by Laura Koenig, then at the University of Minnesota, and her
colleagues, for example, the researchers analyzed reports on the
religiosity of twins in adolescence compared with adulthood. The intent
was to calculate the relative importance of genetic factors versus
environmental influence at those two stages of life. The scientists used
a statistical model to determine which factor is most important in
adolescence versus adulthood. For adolescents, they learned that
genetics—in
other words, dispositions for certain personality traits—accounted for
only 12 percent of their religious identity, and a shared upbringing
contributed 56 percent to the outcome. (If you include a third category,
which captures all the unique events that shape a twin’s life, these
three numbers add up to 100.) Conversely, 44 percent of adults’
religiosity could be attributed to genetics, and 18 percent had to do
with their environment.
All these data suggest that genetic influences help to explain why
adults sometimes stray from the beliefs of their childhood. The more
distance they get from the influences of their early years, the more
idiosyncratic factors can hold sway over a person’s attitudes. In a way,
we are born to be inclined toward religion or atheism. Does God call
us? For some of us, the answer is yes: through our genes, parents,
acquaintances and life events.
This is article was published in print as "Are We Born to Be Religious?"